Noncontingent Reinforcement
Study: Van Camp et al. (2000)
Summary
Noncontingent reinforcement is a function-based treatment for problem behavior that consists of (a) identifying the reinforcer maintaining problem behavior and (b) delivering that reinforcer independent of problem behavior (usually according to a fixed or variable time schedule).
- Target Grades:
- K, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12
- Target Populations:
-
- Students with disabilities only
- Students with learning disabilities
- Students with intellectual disabilities
- Students with emotional or behavioral disabilities
- Any student at risk for emotional and/or behavioral difficulties
- Other: autism spectrum disorders
- Area(s) of Focus:
-
- Physical Aggression
- Property Destruction
- Noncompliance
- High Levels of Disengagement
- Disruptive Behavior
- Other: self-injury
- Where to Obtain:
- N/A
- Initial Cost:
- Free
- Replacement Cost:
- Contact vendor for pricing details.
-
No costs are associated with implementation of noncontingent reinforcement. All that is required for implementation is student-specific reinforcers (e.g., adult attention, preferred items/activities) and a timing device.
- Staff Qualified to Administer Include:
-
- Special Education Teacher
- General Education Teacher
- Reading Specialist
- Math Specialist
- EL Specialist
- Interventionist
- Student Support Services Personnel (e.g., counselor, social worker, school psychologist, etc.)
- Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) Therapist or Board Certified Behavior Analyst (BCBA)
- Paraprofessional
- Other: Clinical therapists/staff
- Training Requirements:
- Training less than 1 hour
-
Training procedures were not consistently described, though likely include (a) a brief explanation of the rationale for NCR, (b) review of any programmed procedures to avoid accidental reinforcement of inappropriate behavior (e.g., 10-s delay to reinforcer delivery if problem behavior occurs when the timer goes off), and (c) practice implementing NCR with a timing/cueing device.
N/A
- Access to Technical Support:
- Not available
- Recommended Administration Formats Include:
-
- Individual students
- Minimum Number of Minutes Per Session:
- Minimum Number of Sessions Per Week:
- Minimum Number of Weeks:
- Detailed Implementation Manual or Instructions Available:
- No
- Is Technology Required?
-
- Other technology: The only technology required, if any, is some form of timing/cuing device (e.g., Motivaider, stopwatch, timer on mobile device).
Program Information
Descriptive Information
Please provide a description of program, including intended use:
Noncontingent reinforcement is a function-based treatment for problem behavior that consists of (a) identifying the reinforcer maintaining problem behavior and (b) delivering that reinforcer independent of problem behavior (usually according to a fixed or variable time schedule).
The program is intended for use in the following age(s) and/or grade(s).
Age 3-5
Kindergarten
First grade
Second grade
Third grade
Fourth grade
Fifth grade
Sixth grade
Seventh grade
Eighth grade
Ninth grade
Tenth grade
Eleventh grade
Twelth grade
The program is intended for use with the following groups.
Students with learning disabilities
Students with intellectual disabilities
Students with emotional or behavioral disabilities
English language learners
Any student at risk for academic failure
Any student at risk for emotional and/or behavioral difficulties
Other
If other, please describe:
autism spectrum disorders
ACADEMIC INTERVENTION: Please indicate the academic area of focus.
Early Literacy
Alphabet knowledge
Phonological awareness
Phonological awarenessEarly writing
Early decoding abilities
Other
If other, please describe:
Language
Grammar
Syntax
Listening comprehension
Other
If other, please describe:
Reading
Phonics/word study
Comprehension
Fluency
Vocabulary
Spelling
Other
If other, please describe:
Mathematics
Concepts and/or word problems
Whole number arithmetic
Comprehensive: Includes computation/procedures, problem solving, and mathematical concepts
Algebra
Fractions, decimals (rational number)
Geometry and measurement
Other
If other, please describe:
Writing
Spelling
Sentence construction
Planning and revising
Other
If other, please describe:
BEHAVIORAL INTERVENTION: Please indicate the behavior area of focus.
Externalizing Behavior
Verbal Threats
Property Destruction
Noncompliance
High Levels of Disengagement
Disruptive Behavior
Social Behavior (e.g., Peer interactions, Adult interactions)
Other
If other, please describe:
self-injury
Internalizing Behavior
Anxiety
Social Difficulties (e.g., withdrawal)
School Phobia
Other
If other, please describe:
Acquisition and cost information
Where to obtain:
- Address
- Phone Number
- Website
Initial cost for implementing program:
- Cost
- $0.00
- Unit of cost
Replacement cost per unit for subsequent use:
- Cost
- Unit of cost
- Duration of license
Additional cost information:
Describe basic pricing plan and structure of the program. Also, provide information on what is included in the published program, as well as what is not included but required for implementation (e.g., computer and/or internet access)
No costs are associated with implementation of noncontingent reinforcement. All that is required for implementation is student-specific reinforcers (e.g., adult attention, preferred items/activities) and a timing device.Program Specifications
Setting for which the program is designed.
Small group of students
BI ONLY: A classroom of students
If group-delivered, how many students compose a small group?
Program administration time
- Minimum number of minutes per session
- Minimum number of sessions per week
- Minimum number of weeks
- If intervention program is intended to occur over less frequently than 60 minutes a week for approximately 8 weeks, justify the level of intensity:
Does the program include highly specified teacher manuals or step by step instructions for implementation?- No
BEHAVIORAL INTERVENTION: Is the program affiliated with a broad school- or class-wide management program?- No
-
If yes, please identify and describe the broader school- or class-wide management program: -
Does the program require technology? - Yes
-
If yes, what technology is required to implement your program? -
Computer or tablet
Internet connection
Other technology (please specify)
If your program requires additional technology not listed above, please describe the required technology and the extent to which it is combined with teacher small-group instruction/intervention:
The only technology required, if any, is some form of timing/cuing device (e.g., Motivaider, stopwatch, timer on mobile device).
Training
- How many people are needed to implement the program ?
- 1
Is training for the instructor or interventionist required?- Yes
- If yes, is the necessary training free or at-cost?
Describe the time required for instructor or interventionist training:- Training less than 1 hour
Describe the format and content of the instructor or interventionist training:- Training procedures were not consistently described, though likely include (a) a brief explanation of the rationale for NCR, (b) review of any programmed procedures to avoid accidental reinforcement of inappropriate behavior (e.g., 10-s delay to reinforcer delivery if problem behavior occurs when the timer goes off), and (c) practice implementing NCR with a timing/cueing device.
What types or professionals are qualified to administer your program?
General Education Teacher
Reading Specialist
Math Specialist
EL Specialist
Interventionist
Student Support Services Personnel (e.g., counselor, social worker, school psychologist, etc.)
Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) Therapist or Board Certified Behavior Analyst (BCBA)
Paraprofessional
Other
If other, please describe:
Clinical therapists/staff- Does the program assume that the instructor or interventionist has expertise in a given area?
-
No
If yes, please describe:
Are training manuals and materials available?- No
-
Describe how the training manuals or materials were field-tested with the target population of instructors or interventionist and students: - N/A
Do you provide fidelity of implementation guidance such as a checklist for implementation in your manual?- No
-
Can practitioners obtain ongoing professional and technical support? -
No
If yes, please specify where/how practitioners can obtain support:
Summary of Evidence Base
- Please identify, to the best of your knowledge, all the research studies that have been conducted to date supporting the efficacy of your program, including studies currently or previously submitted to NCII for review. Please provide citations only (in APA format); do not include any descriptive information on these studies. NCII staff will also conduct a search to confirm that the list you provide is accurate.
-
Austin, J. L., & Soeda, J. M. (2008). Fixed-time teacher attention to decrease off-task behaviors of typically developing third graders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 279-283.
Butler, L. R., & Luiselli, J. K. (2007). Escape-maintained problem behavior in a child with autism: Antecedent functional analysis and intervention evaluation of noncontingent escape and instructional fading. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 9, 195-202.
Hagopian, L. P., Crockett, J. L., van Stone, M., DeLeon, I. G., & Bowman, L. G. (2000). Effects of noncontingent reinforcement on problem behavior and stimulus engagement: The role of satiation, extinction, and alternative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 433-449.
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1997). Noncontingent presentation of attention and alternative stimuli in the treatment of attention-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 229-237.
Ingvarsson, E. T., Kahng, S., & Hausman, N. L. (2008). Some effects of noncontingent positive reinforcement on multiply controlled problem behavior and compliance in a demand context. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 435-440.
Jones, K. M., Drew, H. A., & Weber, N. L. (2000). Noncontingent peer attention as treatment for disruptive classroom behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 343-346.
Kodak, T., Miltenberger, R. G., & Romaniuk, C. (2003). A comparison of differential reinforcement and noncontingent reinforcement for the treatment of a child’s multiply controlled problem behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 267-278.
Lalli, J. S., Casey, S. D., & Kates, K. (1997). Noncontingent reinforcement as treatment for severe problem behavior: Some procedural variations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 127-137.
Lomas, J. E., Fisher, W. W., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). The effects of variable-time delivery of food items and praise on problem behavior reinforced by escape. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 425-435.
Nolan, J. D., & Filter, K. J. (2012). A function-based classroom behavior intervention using non-contingent reinforcement plus response cost. Education and Treatment of Children, 35, 419-430.
Rasmussen, K., & O’Neill, R. E. (2006). The effects of fixed-time reinforcement schedules on problem behavior of children with emotional and behavioral disorders in a day-treatment classroom setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 453-457.
Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., & Sgro, G. M. (2001). Effects of a fixed-time schedule on aberrant and adaptive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 333-336.
Tomlin, M., & Reed, P. (2012). Effects of fixed-time reinforcement delivered by teachers for reducing problem behavior in special education classrooms. Journal of Behavioral Education, 21, 150-162.
Van Camp, C. M., Lerman, D. C., Kelley, M. E., Contrucci, S. A., & Vorndran, C. M. (2000). Variable-time reinforcement schedules in the treatment of socially maintained problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 545-557.
Study Information
Study Citations
Van Camp, C. M., Lerman, D. C., Kelley, M. E., Contrucci, S. A. & Vorndran, C. M. (2000). Variable-time reinforcement schedules in the treatment of socially maintained problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33() 545-557.
Participants
- Describe how students were selected to participate in the study:
- Participants had been referred by teachers and parents for the assessment and treatment of severe behavior problems (aggression and/or self-injury).
-
Describe how students were identified as being at risk for academic failure (AI) or as having emotional/behavioral difficulties (BI): - Participants had been diagnosed with moderate to severe intellectual disability and were referred by teachers and parents for the assessment and treatment of severe behavior problems (aggression and/or self-injury). Aggression was defined as hitting, pinching, kicking, or pushing the therapist. Self-injury was defined as forceful contact between one or both hands and any part of the head.
-
ACADEMIC INTERVENTION: What percentage of participants were at risk, as measured by one or more of the following criteria:- below the 30th percentile on local or national norm, or
- identified disability related to the focus of the intervention?
- %
-
BEHAVIORAL INTERVENTION: What percentage of participants were at risk, as measured by one or more of the following criteria:- emotional disability label,
- placed in an alternative school/classroom,
- non-responsive to Tiers 1 and 2, or
- designation of severe problem behaviors on a validated scale or through observation?
- %
Provide a description of the demographic and other relevant characteristics of the case used in your study (e.g., student(s), classroom(s)).
Case (Name or number) | Age/Grade | Gender | Race / Ethnicity | Socioeconomic Status | Disability Status | ELL status | Other Relevant Descriptive Characteristics |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
test | test | test | test | test | test | test | test |
Design
- Please describe the study design:
- For Participant 1 (Roger), the relative effects of fixed time and variable time schedules of noncontingent reinforcement were evaluated via a multi-element (alternating treatments) design with an initial baseline condition For Participant 2 (Rachel), a A-B-A-B’ reversal design was used, in which the B condition represented the variable time schedule of noncontingent reinforcement and the B’ condition represented the fixed time schedule of noncontingent reinforcement. (Authors describe design as A-B-A-C.)
Clarify and provide a detailed description of the treatment in the submitted program/intervention:- Participant 1 (Roger): During the treatment analysis, fixed time (FT) and variable time (VT) sessions were conducted each day, and the order of conditions alternated across days. During both FT and VT sessions, the reinforcer (i.e., dish scrubber) was delivered for 20 s, and aggression no longer produced access to the reinforcer (i.e., extinction). Initial schedules were determined by calculating the mean latency to the first target behavior that followed each reinforcer removal during baseline sessions, and these mean latencies were averaged across the last three baseline sessions. Roger’s initial FT and VT schedules were set at 14 s and 22 s, respectively. Subsequent schedules for each treatment session were determined by calculating the mean latency of responding during the three preceding sessions. The FT and VT schedules were adjusted individually based on responding that occurred under the corresponding treatment condition. If no aggression occurred between reinforcer deliveries, the latency for that interval was set at twice the length of the interval. Thus, occurrences of aggression between scheduled reinforcer deliveries served to decrease the schedule for the following sessions, whereas nonresponding between scheduled reinforcer deliveries increased (thinned) the schedule. Participant 2 (Rachel): Rachel’s FT and VT schedules were predetermined and were not dependent on responding in baseline. The VT schedule was implemented during the first treatment phase, with the initial schedule at 100% reinforcement (Rachel had access to her toys for the entire session). The schedule was then thinned from 100% to VT 1 min by systematically decreasing the amount of time the reinforcer was available within each minute from 100% to 33.3% (VT 10 s, VT 20 s, VT 30 s, VT 40 s, VT 1 min). The schedule was then increased by 30-s increments, and the reinforcement access time remained at 20 s until the terminal schedule of 5 min was reached. The criterion for increasing the schedule was two consecutive sessions with responding below 0.7 responses per minute (representing an 80% decrease below mean baseline levels). The criterion for decreasing the schedule was three consecutive sessions with responding at or above mean baseline levels (3.5 responses per minute), but this never occurred. The VT phase terminated when three consecutive sessions with responding at or below 0.7 responses per minute were observed under VT 5 min. The initial schedule and schedule thinning procedures for the FT condition were identical to those used for the VT condition; however, the reinforcer was delivered at regular intervals. *Reinforcers were identified based on results of previous functional analyses for each participant (i.e., highest rates of problem behavior occurred during tangible condition) and results of paired choice preference assessments.
Clarify what procedures occurred during the control/baseline condition (third, competing conditions are not considered; if you have a third, competing condition [e.g., multi-element single subject design with a third comparison condition], in addition to your control condition, identify what the competing condition is [data from this competing condition will not be used]):- Participant 1 (Roger): Baseline sessions were identical to the tangible condition of Roger’s functional analysis and were 15 minutes in duration. Specifically, 20 s of access to leisure materials was delivered contingent on aggression. Participant 2 (Rachel): Baseline sessions were identical to the tangible condition of Rachel’s functional analysis and were 10 minutes in duration. Specifically, 20 s of access to leisure materials was delivered contingent on aggression or self-injury. All sessions (baseline and NCR conditions) were conducted in unused rooms at the participants’ schools.
Please describe how replication of treatment effect was demonstrated (e.g., reversal or withdrawal of intervention, across participants, across settings)- For Participant 1 (Roger), an expected treatment effect was to be demonstrated via rapid alternation of FT and VT conditions. However, because there was no consistent differentiation between these conditions, the design only allows one demonstration of effect from the initial baseline to comparison phase. For Participant 2 (Rachel), replication of treatment effect is demonstrated via introduction and withdrawal of noncontingent reinforcement (VT and FT schedules).
-
Please indicate whether (and how) the design contains at least three demonstrations of experimental control (e.g., ABAB design, multiple baseline across three or more participants). - For Participant 1 (Roger), there is only one demonstration of experimental control (initial baseline to treatment comparison condition). For Participant 2 (Rachel), three demonstrations of experimental control are identified via A-B-A-B’ design.
If the study is a multiple baseline, is it concurrent or non-concurrent?- N/A
Fidelity of Implementation
- How was the program delivered?
-
Individually
Small Group
Classroom
If small group, answer the following:
- Average group size
- Minimum group size
- Maximum group size
What was the duration of the intervention (If duration differed across participants, settings, or behaviors, describe for each.)?
- Weeks
- Sessions per week
- 15.50
- Duration of sessions in minutes
- 14.50
- Weeks
- Sessions per week
- 15.50
- Duration of sessions in minutes
- 10.00
- Weeks
- Sessions per week
- Duration of sessions in minutes
- What were the background, experience, training, and ongoing support of the instructors or interventionists?
- No information is provided.
Describe when and how fidelity of treatment information was obtained.- Therapist behaviors (attention, escape, and toy delivery) were collected using duration recording. Treatment integrity was assessed by calculating the difference (in seconds) between the scheduled time of each reinforcer delivery and the actual time of each reinforcer delivery. Each difference score was subtracted from the scheduled reinforcer delivery time, and the result was divided by the scheduled time and multiplied by 100% to express treatment integrity for that reinforcer delivery. Treatment integrity for each session was determined by calculating the average integrity across reinforcer deliveries. Treatment integrity was calculated for 34% of treatment sessions for each participant.
What were the results on the fidelity-of-treatment implementation measure?- Treatment integrity for Roger was 93.6% (range, 79.7-99.3%) for the fixed time condition and 98.1% (range, 94-99.3%) for the variable time condition. Treatment integrity for Rachel was 96.1% (range, 77-100%) for the fixed time condition and 95.6% (range, 78-99.8%) for the variable time condition.
Was the fidelity measure also used in baseline or comparison conditions?- The treatment integrity measure was only reported for each treatment condition (fixed time schedule and variable time schedule) and was not reported for the baseline condition.
Measures and Results
Measures Broader :
Study measures are classified as targeted, broader, or administrative data according to the following definitions:
-
Targeted measures
Assess outcomes, such as competencies or skills, that the program was directly targeted to improve.- In the academic domain, targeted measures typically are not the very items taught but rather novel items structured similarly to the content addressed in the program. For example, if a program taught word-attack skills, a targeted measure would be decoding of pseudo words. If a program taught comprehension of cause-effect passages, a targeted measure would be answering questions about cause-effect passages structured similarly to those used during intervention, but not including the very passages used for intervention.
- In the behavioral domain, targeted measures evaluate aspects of external or internal behavior the program was directly targeted to improve and are operationally defined.
-
Broader measures
Assess outcomes that are related to the competencies or skills targeted by the program but not directly taught in the program.- In the academic domain, if a program taught word-level reading skill, a broader measure would be answering questions about passages the student reads. If a program taught calculation skill, a broader measure would be solving word problems that require the same kinds of calculation skill taught in the program.
- In the behavioral domain, if a program taught a specific skill like on-task behavior in one classroom, a broader measure would be on-task behavior in another setting.
- Administrative data measures apply only to behavioral intervention tools and are measures such as office discipline referrals (ODRs) and graduation rates, which do not have psychometric properties as do other, more traditional targeted or broader measures.
Targeted Measure | Reverse Coded? | Evidence | Relevance |
---|---|---|---|
Targeted Measure 1 | Yes | A1 | A2 |
Broader Measure | Reverse Coded? | Evidence | Relevance |
---|---|---|---|
Broader Measure 1 | Yes | A1 | A2 |
Administrative Data Measure | Reverse Coded? | Relevance |
---|---|---|
Admin Measure 1 | Yes | A2 |
- If you have excluded a variable or data that are reported in the study being submitted, explain the rationale for exclusion:
- No additional measures were reported.
Results
- Describe the method of analyses you used to determine whether the intervention condition improved relative to baseline phase (e.g., visual inspection, computation of change score, mean difference):
- Visual inspection
Please present results in terms of within and between phase patterns. Data on the following data characteristics must be included: level, trend, variability, immediacy of the effect, overlap, and consistency of data patterns across similar conditions. Submitting only means and standard deviations for phases is not sufficient. Data must be included for each outcome measure (targeted, broader, and administrative if applicable) that was described above.- Participant 1 (Roger): During the initial baseline condition, rates of aggression were moderately high and variable (1-2 responses per min). When the VT and FT schedules of noncontingent reinforcement were introduced, the level of aggression increased during the first session (FT) then gradually decreased across both NCR conditions, becoming lower and more stable as the condition progressed. Because there was no clear differentiation between the FT and VT NCR schedules, these data represent only one demonstration of effect from the initial baseline to the treatment comparison phase. Participant 2 (Rachel): During the initial baseline condition, rates of problem behavior were high and variable (2-5.5 responses per minute). When the VT schedule was introduced, there was an immediate decrease to zero rates of problem behavior across several sessions. Though rates of problem behavior temporarily increased in this condition, they stabilized near zero by the end of the condition. When the VT schedule was withdrawn, rates of problem behavior increased within in the first 2 sessions, and were variable with a possible increasing trend. When the FT schedule was introduced, rates of problem behavior immediately decreased and gradually reached steady states of zero-rates.
Additional Research
- Is the program reviewed by WWC or E-ESSA?
- No
- Summary of WWC / E-ESSA Findings :
What Works Clearinghouse Review
This program was not reviewed by What Works Clearinghouse.
- How many additional research studies are potentially eligible for NCII review?
- 10
- Citations for Additional Research Studies :
Butler, L. R., & Luiselli, J. K. (2007). Escape-Maintained Problem Behavior in a Child with Autism: Antecedent Functional Analysis and Intervention Evaluation of Noncontingent Escape and Instructional Fading. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 9, 195-202.
Hagopian, L. P., Crockett, J. L., van Stone, M., DeLeon, I. G., & Bowman, L. G. (2000). Effects of Noncontingent Reinforcement on Problem Behavior and Stimulus Engagement: The Role of Satiation, Extinction, and Alternative Reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 433-449.
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1997). Noncontingent Presentation of Attention and Alternative Stimuli in the Treatment of Attention-Maintained Destructive Behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 229-237.
Kodak, T., Miltenberger, R. G., & Romaniuk, C. (2003). A Comparison of Differential Reinforcement and Noncontingent Reinforcement for the Treatment of a Child’s Multiply Controlled Problem Behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 267-278.
Lalli, J. S., Casey, S. D., & Kates, K. (1997). Noncontingent Reinforcement as Treatment for Severe Problem Behavior: Some Procedural Variations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 127-137.
Lomas, J. E., Fisher, W. W., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). The Effects of Variable-Time Delivery of Food Items and Praise on Problem Behavior Reinforced by Escape. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 425-435.
Rasmussen, K., & O’Neill, R. E. (2006). The Effects of Fixed-Time Reinforcement Schedules on Problem Behavior of Children with Emotional and Behavioral Disorders in a Day-Treatment Classroom Setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 453-457.
Tomlin, M., & Reed, P. (2012). Effects of Fixed-Time Reinforcement Delivered by Teachers for Reducing Problem Behavior in Special Education Classrooms. Journal of Behavioral Education, 21, 150-162.
Van Camp, C. M., Lerman, D. C., Kelley, M. E., Contrucci, S. A., & Vorndran, C. M. (2000). Variable-Time Reinforcement Schedules in the Treatment of Socially Maintained Problem Behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 545-557.
Waller, R. D., & Higbee, T. S. (2010). The Effects of Fixed-Time Escape on Inappropriate and Appropriate Classroom Behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 149-153.
Data Collection Practices
Most tools and programs evaluated by the NCII are branded products which have been submitted by the companies, organizations, or individuals that disseminate these products. These entities supply the textual information shown above, but not the ratings accompanying the text. NCII administrators and members of our Technical Review Committees have reviewed the content on this page, but NCII cannot guarantee that this information is free from error or reflective of recent changes to the product. Tools and programs have the opportunity to be updated annually or upon request.